• Shuffle
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Alphabetize
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Front First
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Both Sides
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Read
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
Reading...
Front

Card Range To Study

through

image

Play button

image

Play button

image

Progress

1/54

Click to flip

Use LEFT and RIGHT arrow keys to navigate between flashcards;

Use UP and DOWN arrow keys to flip the card;

H to show hint;

A reads text to speech;

54 Cards in this Set

  • Front
  • Back
Accidental property
A property of objects that only occurs when viewing the object from a specific, rarely encountered, viewpoint. For example, although three sides of a solid cube are visible from most viewpoints, an accidental property occurs when the cube is viewed end-on, so only one side is visible.
Algorithm
A procedure that is guaranteed to result in the solution to a problem. For example, the procedures we learn for addition, subtraction, and long division are algorithms.
Apparent movement (or stroboscopic movement)
An illusion of movement that occurs between two objects separated in space when the objects are flashed rapidly on and off, one after another, separated by a brief time interval.
Border ownership
When two areas share a border, as occurs in figure–ground displays, the border is usually perceived as belonging to the figure.
Common fate, law of
A Gestalt law of perceptual organization that states that things that are moving in the same direction appear to be grouped together.
Common region, principle of
A modern Gestalt principle that states that elements that are within the same region of space appear to be grouped together.
Componential recovery, principle of
A principle of the recognition-by-components model that states that we can rapidly and correctly identify an object if we can perceive its individual geons.
Contextual modulation
When the neural response to a stimulus is influenced by the context within which the stimulus occurs. This term has been used to refer to the situation in which a neuron’s response is influenced by stimulation of an area outside its receptive field.
Delayed-matching-to-sample procedure
A procedure in which a sample stimulus is presented, followed by a delay period, followed by a test period. During the test period, the same stimulus or a different one is presented. The task is to indicate whether the test stimulus is the same as or different from the sample stimulus.
Discriminability
Generally, the ability to distinguish between one stimulus and another. In the recognition-by-components theory of object perception, discriminability is a property of geons, which indicates that each geon can be distinguished from other geons from almost all viewpoints.
Familiarity, law of
A Gestalt law of perceptual organization that states that things are more likely to form groups when the groups appear familiar or meaningful.
Feedback (nervous system)
When signals are transmitted “backward” from “higher” areas, such as the temporal or parietal lobe, to “earlier” areas such as the primary visual receiving area in the occipital lobe.
Figure
When an object is seen as separate from the background (the “ground”), it is called a figure. See also Figure-ground segregation.
Figure–ground segregation
The perceptual separation of an object from its background.
Geon
The volumetric features of Biederman’s recognition-by-components theory of object perception.
Gestalt psychology
An approach to psychology that focuses on developing principles of perceptual organization, proposing that “the whole differs from the sum of its parts.”
Good continuation, law of
A Gestalt law of perceptual organization that states that points that, when connected, result in straight or smoothly curving lines are seen as belonging together, and lines tend to be seen in such a way as to follow the smoothest path.
Good figure, law of
See Pragnanz, law of.
Ground
In object perception, the background is called the ground. See also Figure.
Heuristic
In perception, a rule of thumb that provides a “best guess” estimate of the identity of a particular stimulus.
Illusory contour
Contour that is perceived even though they are not present in the physical stimulus.
Image-description model
-ability to recognize objects from different viewpoints is based on

stored two-dimensional views

of the object as it would appear from different viewpoints.
Inverse projection problem
The idea that a particular image on the retina could have been caused by an infinite number of different objects. Thus, the retinal image does not unambiguously specify a stimulus.
Laws of perceptual organization
See Perceptual organization, laws of.
Light-from-above heuristic
The assumption that light is usually coming from above, which influences our perception of form in some situations.
Likelihood principle
The idea proposed by Helmholtz that we perceive the object that is most likely to have caused the pattern of stimuli we have received.
Masking stimulus
A visual pattern that, when presented immediately after a visual stimulus decreases a person’s ability to perceive the stimulus. This stops the persistence of vision and therefore limits the effective duration of the stimulus.
Morphing
A computerized method of combining two stimuli which involves locating corresponding points on two “prototype” stimuli and combining the features that exist at these points in different amounts.
Nearness, law of
See Proximity, law of.
Oblique effect
Enhanced sensitivity to vertically and horizontally oriented visual stimuli. This effect has been demonstrated by measuring both perception and neural responding.
Perceptual organization
The process by which small elements become perceptually grouped into larger objects.
Perceptual organization, laws of
"laws" of Gestalt psychology that specify how we organize small parts into wholes.

laws:
-common fate
-familiarity
-good continuation
-good figure
-nearness
-similarity

modern researchers have proposed some additional laws.
Perceptual segregation
Perceptual organization in which one object is seen as separate from other objects.
Persistence of vision
A phenomenon in which perception of any stimulus persists for about 250 ms after the stimulus is physically terminated.
Pragnanz, law of
A Gestalt law of perceptual organization that states that every stimulus pattern is seen in such a way that the resulting structure is as simple as possible. Also called the law of good figure or the law of simplicity.
Principle of componential recovery
See Componential recovery, principle of.
Proximity, law of
A Gestalt law of perceptual organization that states that things that are near to each other appear to be grouped together. Also called the law of nearness.
Recognition-by-components (RBC) theory
A theory of object perception proposed by Biederman, which proposes that we recognize objects by decomposing them into volumetric features called geons. See also Structural-description models.
Region-of-interest (ROI) approach
A procedure used in brain imaging in which subjects are pretested on the stimuli to be studied. This enables researchers to establish the precise location in the brain that they will be studying for each individual person.
Regularities in the environment
Characteristics of the environment that occur regularly and in many different situations.
Reversible figure–ground
A figure–ground pattern that perceptually reverses as it is viewed, so that the figure becomes the ground and the ground becomes the figure. The most well-known reversible figure–ground pattern is Rubin’s vase–face pattern.
Sensations
Elementary elements that according to the structuralists, combine to create perceptions.
Similarity, law of
A Gestalt law stating that similar things appear to be grouped together.
Simplicity, law of
See Pragnanz, law of.
Stroboscopic movement
See Apparent movement.
Structural-description model
An approach to object perception that states that our ability to recognize three-dimensional objects is based on three-dimensional volumes that can be combined to create the overall shape of the object. This approach is associated with Biederman’s recognition-by-components theory of object perception. For another approach, see Image-description models.
Structuralism
The approach to psychology, prominent in the late 19th and early 20th centuries, that postulated that perceptions result from the summation of many elementary sensations. The Gestalt approach to perception was, in part, a reaction to structuralism.
Synchrony, principle of
A modern principle of perceptual organization that states that visual events that occur at the same time will be perceived as belonging together.
Theory of unconscious inference
The idea proposed by Helmholtz that some of our perceptions are the result of unconscious assumptions that we make about the environment. See also Likelihood principle.
Trichromat
A person with normal color vision. Trichromats can match any wavelength in the spectrum by mixing three other wavelengths in various proportions.
Uniform connectedness, principle of
A modern Gestalt principle that states that connected regions of a visual stimulus are perceived as a single unit.
View-invariant properties
Properties of objects that remain visible even when an object is viewed from different angles. For example, a view-invariant property of a rectangular solid is its three parallel edges, which are visible from most viewing angles.
Viewpoint invariance
Objects that have properties that don’t change when viewed from different angles. The geons in the recognition-by-components theory of object perception are view invariant.
Volumetric features
Three-dimensional features that can be combined to create objects. The geons of the recognition-by-components theory of recognition are volumetric features.