• Shuffle
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Alphabetize
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Front First
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Both Sides
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Read
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
Reading...
Front

Card Range To Study

through

image

Play button

image

Play button

image

Progress

1/51

Click to flip

Use LEFT and RIGHT arrow keys to navigate between flashcards;

Use UP and DOWN arrow keys to flip the card;

H to show hint;

A reads text to speech;

51 Cards in this Set

  • Front
  • Back

Factors involved in Personality

1. Genetic Influences: Our genes help to shape our interests and personality


2. Environmental factors:


a) Shared Environment: Things that make us tend to be like others who we share an environment with


b) Non-Shared Environment: Things we experience uniquely that shape us differently from everyone else

Phenotype

Phenotypic characteristic is any measurable or observable aspect of who we are i.e. hair, personality



Phenotype is the set of all these measurable and observable aspects of us



Genotype is that set of genes we inherit from our parents

Twin Studies

Monozygotic (MZ) twins develop from a single fertilized egg and are therefore genetically identical (100%)


Dizygotic (DZ) twins who develop from two simaltaneously fertilized eggs, and are therefore no more similar in genotype than any pair of siblings (50%)

Twin Studies: Reared Together and Apart
IQ as an Example


If the correlation for IQ is 100% is genetically determined then the twins raised together or apart will have a correlation of 1.00


For DZ twins the correlation is .50

Family Studies

Family Studies

Since studies of heritability only require that we compare the phenotupic similarity of people with different degrees of genetic similarity,we need not use twins in our studies

Adoption Studies

Adoption Studies

Adopted children have no genetic overlap with their adoptive parents


If a phenotypic characteristic is 100% determined by genes, we expect there to be essentially no correlation between children and their adoption parents, but a 50% correlation between children and their biological parents

Problems with Studies

DZ twins confused as MZ twins at birth


Problems with Studies:
Selective Placement

We make an assumption that MZ twins reared apart live in different environments



However when twins are reared apart, adoption agency tries to match the twins to similar families so there would be similarities

Problem with Studies:


Differences between MZ and DZ twins in environmental similarity

We assume that MZ and DZ twins are reared in the same environments



Parents of MZ often create very similar environments



Parents of DZ often create different environments to rear them


-This overestimates the influence of genes

Problem with Studies:
Assertive Mating

Problem with Studies:
Assertive Mating

We choose our mates based on genetic similarity


But we assume that parents contirbute 50% each to the child's genetic makeup. So in this case, when the mother and father shar emore genes in common and the child inherits those genes, it is perceived that there is a great contribution from one of the parents


This overestimates the influence of genes

Distributions and Variance

The variance of a distirbution of numbers is a measure of the varibility of those scores around the mean (or average) score

Components of Variance

Components of Variance

(1) Variance due to genetic differences between the people who provided the scores in our distribution


(2) Variance due to differences in the environments of the people who provided our scores.

Heritability Coefficient

Heritability Coefficient

The heritability coefficient for some phenotypic characteristic is the ratio of the genetic variance (Vg) to the total variance (Vt)

Formulas for the Heritability Coefficient

Formulas for the Heritability Coefficient

(1) Used when the data available consists of correlations between pairs of monozygotic twins reared apart (MZA)


(2) Used for calculating the heritability coefficient when the data consists of DZ twins reared apart (DZA)


(3) Used when the data consists of both correlations between pairs of MZ twins, and pairs of DZ twins

Heritability Coefficient: What it isn't
Applies only to groups, not individuals

Heritability Coefficient is about where the variance in a group or population comes from


I.e. all the variance in IQ in the population half of that is attributable to environment and half attributable to genetics

Heritability Coefficient: What it isn't


Varies from popul'n to popul'n, over time

Heritability coefficient is not a constant.


If the genetic pool in Sweden is smaller, the genetic variance will be lower and therefore the heritability coefficient will be higher.

Heritability Coefficient: What it isn't


Valid only if measures used to calculate it are valid

Twin studis are often invalid because of assumptions we make

Heritability Quotient

The heritability Quotient merely tells how much of the total variability in some phenotypic characteristic can be attributed to genetic variability within our sample

Thomas, Chess & Birch (1970)

The study by Thomas, Chess and Birch was one of the first attempts to define early measures of temperament.


They assessed infants

Thomas, Chess & Birch (1970)

Positive response to new object.


Regularity in biological functions


Friendliness or good mood


Adapts to changes in environment


Usual Degree of energy in responding(intensity)


Thomas, Chess & Birch: Easy

Regular rhytms


Good mood


Accepts novelty


Adapts well to change


Low to moderate levels of energy

Thomas, Chess & Birch: Difficult

Irregular rhythms


Poor mood


Dislikes novelty


Adapts poorly to change


High levels of energy

Thomas, Chess & Birch: What they found

It was found that most of the kids had a high correlation of thei rtemeprament forma ge 1 to age 14.


They also showed how being Easy or difficult had an effect ontheir mental wellbeing.

Plomin & Rowe (1979)

The Plomin & Rowe study examined 21-month-old MZ and Dz twin for their similarity in several aspects of sociability in a situation in which they interacted with a stranger.


-Looking at stranger


-Approaching stranger


-Nearness to Stranger


-Vocalizing to Stranger

Plomin & Rowe: The results

The contrast effect in which DZ correlations ar emcuh smaller than they should be given the relative genetic similarity of MZ and DZ twins

Buss & Plomin (1984)

Buss & Plomin developed a 20-item EAS survey (taken by the parents) to evaluate children on the three important dimension of temperament. Their survey of MZ and Dz twins found high heritability coefficients for all three, but complicated by the same sort of contrast effect that Plomin & Rowe had reported five years earlier

Buss & Plomin: The 3 Dimensions of Temperament

Emotionality


Activity


Sociability

Why a Contrast Effect?: Non-Additive Variance

The data assumes 100% that genes in your genotype show up in your phenotupe, but this isn't the case

Non-Additive Variance

Dominant-recessive effects: What shows up in th ephenotype depend sonw hat particular allele is dominant int he genotype.


MZ have the same alleles but DZ twins could have different ones

Non-Additive Variance

Epistatic effects: When one gene somewhere in a genotype affects the expression of one of them.


Once again, MZ twins share 100% of both additive and non-additive genetic effects.


Dz twins share 50% of additive effects but only 25% of non-additive effects

Why a Contrast Effect?: Epigenetic Effects

The epigenome is a set of chemical instructions for gene expression


It differentiates cell types in organs


It is passed on to offspring, affected by environment and experience

Epigenetic Effects: Environments not equal

We assume that the similarity between the nevironments of DZ twins is the same as the ismilarity in the environments of MZ twins

Environments not Equal: Contrast Effects

Parents of DZ twins often work hard to differentiate their environments to emphasize their uniqueness

Environments not Equal: Assimilation Effects

Parents of MZ twins often work to make their two environments even more similar than they would otherwise be

Jerome Kagan and Behavioural Inhibition:


Identifying Behaviourally Inhibited Children

He did a study and took 117 Caucasian 22-month old boys in playroom:


Mom and examiner join child on floor, act otu behaviours of varying complexity.Examiner leaves, stranger (Female adult) enter, asks child to join her. Examiner returns, reveals noisy robot to child, suggests child approach, touch robot. Examiner leaves, mother leaves-child alone

Jerome Kagan and Behavioural Inhibition: Results

Some show fear in most situations: Inhibited (10%)


Some show no fear in an situation: Uninhibited (20%)


Remainder show mixed reactions: Middling (70%)

Behaviourally Inhibited Children

Slow to start conversation with unfamiliar adult or peer.


Seldom smile spontaneously with unfamiliar other.


Slow to relax in unfamiliar situations.


Memory impaired following stress.


Cautious decision makers (low risk takers).


Unusual fears or phobias.

Physiology of Inhibited Children

HR up during stress, or when stand up


Diastolic BP up when stand up (more dramatic)


More rapid pupillary dilation under stress


Higher muscle tension


Higher right frontal cortex activation


More allergies


More activity in HPA axis, RAS, ANS (sympathetic branch)

Inhibition and Somatotype

Kagan reported several intriguing relationshups between behavioural inhibition and several other aspects of appearance and body shape


60% of inhibited children have blue eyes


60% of uninhibited children have brown eyes


More kin of inhibited children have blue eyes; of uninhibited have brown


Inhibited boys have narrow faces, thin builds

Predicting Inhibition

Loehlin & Nicholas (1976)

A study by Loehlin and Nichols looked at the correlations between DZ and MZ twins on a number of personality variables


Studied 850 pairs of same-sex twins who took the NMST in 1962


514 MZ pairs and 336 DZ pairs


41% males and 58% females


Filled out personality & interest tests; parents completed questionnaires

Loehlin & Nichols: Results

Loehlin & Nichols: Results

Notice the high heritability coefficients for male twins, and the presence of large contrast effects in the male twin data. L & N concluded that the average heritability coefficient for personality characteristics was about 50%

Tellegen et al (1988)

They took the data of one of the studies and measured the correlation coefficients of twins reared apart

They took the data of one of the studies and measured the correlation coefficients of twins reared apart

Scarr et al (1981)

Compared two groups of families:


1. 234 biological children (mean age 18 years) in 120 families


2. 194 adopted children (mean age 18 years) un 115 families

Scarr et al: They measured...

Introversion


SOcialibility


Anxiety


Emotionality

Scarr et al

Heritability estimates for personality:


.22 (parent-child similarity)


.26 (sibling-sibling similarity)

Swedish Adoption/Twin Study on Aging (SATSA)

With MZA, DZA, MZT and DZT twins, they were able to calculate heritability using a variety of different formulas

Shortcomings of h2 Calculations

Ignores environmental effects that bias calculations


All the studies assume only additive genetic variance; non-additive variance is assumed as zero


No separate calculation shared vs non-shared environmental affects

SATSA Variances

The SATSA authors were able to separately estimate the amount of variance attributable to the several different factors that contribute to phenotypic variability

SATSA VAriances:

Genetics 29%


Shared Environment 9%


Non-Shared Environment and Error 62%

Non-Shared Environmental Effects

Differential treatment by parents


Different interaction with siblings


Birth order


Sibling age spacing


Gender differences


Different school experiences


Different friends, playmates