• Shuffle
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Alphabetize
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Front First
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Both Sides
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Read
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
Reading...
Front

Card Range To Study

through

image

Play button

image

Play button

image

Progress

1/60

Click to flip

Use LEFT and RIGHT arrow keys to navigate between flashcards;

Use UP and DOWN arrow keys to flip the card;

H to show hint;

A reads text to speech;

60 Cards in this Set

  • Front
  • Back
normal eyeball
emmetropia
short eye ball
farsightedness-hyperopia
long eyeball
nearsightedness-myopia
photopigments
which break down in presence of light

Rods contain rhodopsin

Cones contain iodopsin
Rods
contain rhodopsin
Rhodopsin is extremely sensitive to light.
Rods function well in dim light, poorly in bright light
Rods do not distinguish color
In peripheral retina
Rhodopsin
Rhodopsin is extremely sensitive to light.
Rods function well in dim light, poorly in bright light

Rods do not distinguish color
In peripheral retina
Cones
contain iodopsin
Iodopsin requires bright light to function
Cones do well in daylight, not dim light
3 types of iodopsin allow cones to respond to different wavelengths
In central retina (fovea)
Iodopsin
Iodopsin requires bright light to function
Cones do well in daylight, not dim light
3 types of iodopsin allow cones to respond to different wavelengths
In central retina (fovea)
cones are in central fovea
rods are in peripheral retina
Receptive fields of rods are large
Receptive fields of cones are small
Receptive fields of rods are large
Receptive fields of rods are large
Many rods share each ganglion cell
This enhances their already greater sensitivity to light
But it reduces their acuity
Receptive fields of cones are small
Receptive fields of cones are small
Few cones attach to each ganglion cell; in the fovea each cone has its own ganglion cell
Thus, visual acuity - ability to distinguish details - is high
rods
Rods contain rhodopsin
Rhodopsin is extremely sensitive to light.
Rods function well in dim light, poorly in bright light
Rods do not distinguish color
In peripheral retina

Receptive fields of rods are large
Many rods share each ganglion cell
This enhances their already greater sensitivity to light
But it reduces their acuity
Cones
Cones contain iodopsin
Iodopsin requires bright light to function
Cones do well in daylight, not dim light
3 types of iodopsin allow cones to respond to different wavelengths
In central retina (fovea)

Receptive fields of cones are small
Few cones attach to each ganglion cell; in the fovea each cone has its own ganglion cell
Thus, visual acuity - ability to distinguish details - is high
Two types of lateral processing cells
Horizontal cells contact photoreceptors and bipolar cells
Amacrine cells contact bipolar and ganglion cells
Horizontal cells contact photoreceptors and bipolar cells
Amacrine cells contact bipolar and ganglion cells
All cell types except ganglion cells generate graded potentials
ganglion cells fire action potentials
When light strikes rhodopsin
retinal is activated

RPE65
Leber’s congenital optic degeneration
RPE65 is defective; photoreceptors degenerate; patient goes blind
Stem cells have cured this disease
photoreceptor mechanisms
most active when they are not being stimulated by light. In darkness, the photoreceptor's sodium and calcium channels are open, allowing these ions to flow in freely. Thus, the membrane is partially depolarized, the receptor releases a continuous flow of glutamate, and this inhibits activity in the bipolar cells. The chemical response that occurs when light stricks the photopigments closes the sodium and calcium channels, reducing the release of glutamate inproportion to the amount of light. The bipolar cells release more neurotransmitter, which increases the firing rate in the ganglion cells
Transduction
Light closes Na+ channels, hyperpolarizes; turns rods off!

Rods are so sensitive that at high illumination - in sunlight - all the sodium channels are closed - the rods are saturated
Lateral inhibition
– connected bipolar cells – inhibiting one’s neighbors produces contrast
Visual pathway:
Retina
Optic chiasm
Lateral geniculate nucleus (thalamus)
Visual cortex (occipital lobe)
mach band illusions
ganglion cell activated more -lighter band
ganglion cell minimal stimulation-darker band
receptor give neighboring ganglion cells give stimulation or activation to the ganglion cells
two types of ganglion cells
on center off surround

off center on surround

on is where light produces excitation, off produces inhibition

on center cells-light in the center increases firing and light in the off surround reduces firing below the resting level
LGN of the thalamus has three cell types
Parvocellular – four dorsal layers – small cells, small receptive fields
Magnocellular – two ventral layers – large cells, large receptive fields
Koniocellular – layers with very small cells, between main layers
Parvocellular
four dorsal layers – small cells, small receptive fields
Magnocellular
– two ventral layers – large cells, large receptive fields
Koniocellular
layers with very small cells, between main layers
Primary Visual Cortex
Primary visual cortex (V1) in occipital cortex – where most visual information first arrives
Brain maps of visual space are mostly devoted to the fovea
Simple cortical cells
Complex cortical cells
Cells are classified as simple or complex,
depending on their receptive fields
Simple cortical cells
Simple cortical cells – also called bar or edge detectors – respond to an edge or bar of a particular width, orientation, and location
Complex cortical cells
Complex cortical cells – also respond to a bar of a particular width and orientation, but may be located anywhere in the visual field
Simple cells
Simple cells are line detectors. They are part of the recognition/location system. They are more responsive to flashes of light, than to static, steady illumination or diffuse illumination
Complex cells
Complex cells are concerned more with recognition of external objects than with their precise localization.
Simple neurons receive input from neurons in the lateral geniculate;
complex neurons receive input from simple cells
Form Vision Spatial Frequency Theory
Visual cortical cells do a frequency analysis of the luminosity variations in a scene
Different visual cortical cells have different sensitivities, not just those required to detect edges
As a result, visual cortical cells can detect not just edges but grades of luminosity change
High vs. Low Frequencies in Vision
Images with only high frequency transitions are not meaningful. Those with more gradual transitions (low frequencies) are more recognizable

Support for the theory: Cortical cells respond to light-dark “gratings” containing a specific combination of frequencies
V1
(primary visual cortex) perceives objects and is necessary in forming mental images
color
V2, V4
, and the inferior temporal lobe - perceive complex form
V5
is specialized for motion perception
V2 is important for this -
“fills in the gaps
Much of vision
is extrapolating (predicting) from what is actually ‘seen’
V4 cells respond to concentric and radial stimuli
V4 is also involved in color perception
Motion detection
Retinal periphery (rods) sensitive to motion
Cortically, V5 is important
Motion blindness (akinetopsia)
Types of input from LGN to V1 – 3 channels
1. M channel (magnocellular pathway) –
orientation selective, directional sensitive for movement, no color sensitivity - analysis of object motion
2. P-IB channel (parvocellular interblob pathway) –
high orientation sensitivity, no color sensitivity, small receptive fields - analysis of object shape
3. Blob channel (parvocellular blob and koniocellular pathway) -
no orientation sensitivity, color sensitivity - analysis of object color
. M channel (magnocellular pathway) –
orientation selective, directional sensitive for movement, no color sensitivity - analysis of object motion
. P-IB channel (parvocellular interblob pathway) –
high orientation sensitivity, no color sensitivity, small receptive fields - analysis of object shape
Blob channel (parvocellular blob and koniocellular pathway) -
no orientation sensitivity, color sensitivity - analysis of object color
Area V1 Is Organized in Columns and Slabs
An ocular dominance column is a region of cortex with greater synaptic input from one eye
Two hypotheses of color perception
Trichromatic hypothesis:
Three types of cones
Each responds to a part of the spectrum
Each has a separate pathway to the brain

Opponent-process hypothesis:
Three color axes (opposed pairs of colors)
Spectral Sensitivities of Human Photopigments
Supports the trichromatic theory

In the retina
Afterimages
opponent-process theory

When one member of the color pair is "fatigued" by extended inspection, inhibition of its corresponding pair member is reduced. This increases the relative activity level of the unfatigued pair member and so its color is perceived.
Opponent Process:
Cells in the retinal ganglion and thalamic parvocellular layers fire to some wavelengths, and are inhibited by others
Synesthesia
: activation of V4 and fusiform gyrus by spoken words
Alice in Wonderland syndrome
Micropsia, macropsia that alternates

Occurs in migraine, EBV (‘mono’) infection

Common in children
Simultagnosia
Inability to attend to more than a very limited area of the visual field despite normal visual fields

Patients can see individual objects but cannot perceive a visual scene as a whole
Palinopsia
Patients see afterimages, both as a reduced amount of time required to form an afterimage, and an increased duration of the afterimage. Even routine eye movement is accompanied by flickers of what the eye has scanned ("tracers").
Left hemisphere reads words
Right hemisphere reads color
Patient G.Y.: Blindsight is due to minor pathways into extrastriate cortex that bypass V1 (maybe)
Blindsight: patients with damage to primary visual cortex can tell where an object is although they claim they cannot see it