• Shuffle
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Alphabetize
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Front First
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Both Sides
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Read
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
Reading...
Front

Card Range To Study

through

image

Play button

image

Play button

image

Progress

1/40

Click to flip

Use LEFT and RIGHT arrow keys to navigate between flashcards;

Use UP and DOWN arrow keys to flip the card;

H to show hint;

A reads text to speech;

40 Cards in this Set

  • Front
  • Back

Oculomotor Cues

Oculomotor depth cues come from two sources. Convergence and accommodation.

Convergence

Inward movement of the eyes when we look at near objects.

Accommodation

Adjusting the shape of the lens to focus on nearby or far away objects.

Monocular depth cues

Pictorial cues. Occlusion. Relative height. Relative size. Perspective convergence.

Pictorial cues

depth information that can be derived using information present in pictures or scenes.

Occlusion

An object that is partially occluded by another is seen as being farther away

Relative height

Objects that are below the horizon and are higher in the visual field are seen as further away.

Relative size

When two objects are of equal size, the one that is farther away will take up less of your visual field.

Perspective convergence

When parallel lines extend out from an observer they are perceived as converging as distance increases.

Monocular pictorial cues

Familiar size. Atmospheric perspective. Texture gradient. Shadow cues. Motion parallax.

Familiar size

Judging distance based on the known size of objects in the scene.

Atmospheric perspective

More distant objects appear blurry and have a slight bluish colour. The further away an object is the more dust, water particles, smog, etc. we have to look through to see them.

Texture gradient

Elements that are equally spaced in a scene appear to be more closely packed at further distances. Relative size.

Shadow cues

Can also be used to infer the locations of objects within a scene. Shadows can enhance the 3 dimensionality of objects.

Motion parallax

Occurs when objects that are nearby move by us more quickly than objects that are far away.

Binocular depth cues

Your two eyes allow you to see in 3D using binocular cues. Each eye acts as a 'camera' with a slightly different view.

Angle of disparity

Difference between the corresponding point on the retina, and where the image lands on the retina.

Absolute disparity

The absolute difference between the position of the image on the retina and its corresponding point on the retina. The amount of disparity indicates the distance of the object from the horopter. Absolute disparity changes every time we change where we are fixating

Relative disparity

The difference in absolute disparity between two objects in a scene. As longs as objects remain stationary their relative disparities will be the same between saccades

Stereopsis

The impression of depth created through binocular disparity.

Seeing in 3D

Stereopsis. 3D movies present two images on the screen that are taken from cameras at two different viewing angles. Use filters to help you fuse the images together.

Echolocation in bats

Bats emit high frequency sounds and use the reflected sound waves to determine the identity, size, and location of objects.

Physiology of depth perception

recorded from neurons in area CIP while monkeys indicated whether two texture patterns matched. Neurons in area CIP responded to specific depth orientations. The same neurons also respond to disparity signals.

Retinal disparity neurons in V1

V1 contains neurons that are tuned to respond to specific amounts of absolute disparity. These cells compute the disparity between corresponding points of the retina in the two eyes.

Size perception

Size perception and depth perception are inherently linked. We use depth information to make judgements about size.

Visual angle

The angle of an object relative to the eye. The position and size of the object on the retina.

Using depth cues to make size judgements

Participants were asked to match the size of the comparison circle to test circles presented at different distances. Test circles were adjusted to be the same visual angle. If participants use only visual angle then comparison circle should always be adjusted to be the same size as the retinal image size of the test circle.

Results (Holway and Boring): full depth cues

Participants adjusted the comparison circle to be larger as the test circle was presented further away.

Results (Holway and Boring): Monocular viewing

Eliminated disparity signals.

Results (Holway and Boring): Peephole viewing

Eliminates motion parallax.

Results (Holway and Boring): masking the walls

Eliminates shadows and reflections.

Size constancy

The perception of an object's size remains constant despite changes in distance.

Size-distance scaling

S=K(RxD). S = objects perceived size. K = constant. R = retinal image size. D= perceived distance of the object. As an object moves further away R gets smaller, but D gets larger to adjust the perceived size.

Size constancy and Emmert's law

The size of an afterimage depends on the distance of the surface you are looking at. the further away the afterimage appears, the larger it seems.

Familiar size

Estimating the size of something in relation to the size of a familiar object.


Visual illusions

One way to understand size perception is to study how our visual system can be fooled. The illusion results from mis applied size constancy scaling.

Conflicting cues theory

We can still perceive a Muller-Lyer like effect even when corners are not present. Conflicting cues theory suggests that the overall length of one figure is longer, and so we also perceive the line as being longer.

Ponzo Illusion

The ponzo illusion also occurs because of mis applied size constancy scaling. The two objects are exactly the same size, but perspective convergence gives the illusion of depth. If the two objects are the same size on the retina and one is further away, it must be larger.

Actions can be immune to visual illusions

Participants fall victim to the illusion when estimating (ventral stream), but when grasping (dorsal stream).

The Ames room illusion

Also results from size-constancy scaling. When we view the room through a peephole the person on the left has a smaller retinal image size, but depth cues are not available, thus we perceive them as being shorter.