• Shuffle
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Alphabetize
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Front First
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Both Sides
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Read
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
Reading...
Front

Card Range To Study

through

image

Play button

image

Play button

image

Progress

1/40

Click to flip

Use LEFT and RIGHT arrow keys to navigate between flashcards;

Use UP and DOWN arrow keys to flip the card;

H to show hint;

A reads text to speech;

40 Cards in this Set

  • Front
  • Back

epistemology

philosophical study of how we come to have knowledge

Descartes

we learn but there are other sources of knowledge that don't depend on experience

nativism

knowledge that is innately given

rationalism

knowledge derived from a reasoning, logical and intuiting mind

Locke

empiricism and contiguity

empiricism

origin of all knowledge is in experience

contiguous

we associate objects/events together when they occur close together in time or space; frequency, similarity and contrast

learning (formal)

relatively permanent observed change in behavior (repertoire) that occurs as a result of experience

4 parts of learning formal definition

a. an observed change in behavior


b. learning involves changes in behavioral repertoire (the stock of behavior that may be performed)


c. learning occurs as a result of experience


d. learning is said to be relatively permanent

difference between learning and memory

learning refers to the acquisition of knowledge, whereas memory deals with retaining and recalling the acquired knowledge

orienting response (OR)

an automatic response to investigate something (a reflex); occurs when a novel or unexpected stimulus is presented

habituation

the decrease in orienting responses to a stimulus that is repeatedly presented

perceptual learning

we can learn more about a stimulus when it is easily perceived and increased experience with stimuli allows such enhanced perceptions to occur

mere exposure effect

people tend to develop a preference for things merely because they are familiar with them

dishabituation

the fast recovery of a response that has undergone habituation, typically as a result of the presentation of a novel, strong or sometimes noxious stimulus

classical conditioning

the presentation of two (or more) events in an experimentally determined temporal relationship

unconditioned stimulus

an event that consistently and automatically elicits an unconditioned response

unconditioned response

an action that the unconditioned stimulus automatically elicits

conditioned stimulus

initially a neutral stimulus, after repeated pairings with the US, the CS elicits the same response as the US

conditioned response

the response elicited by the conditioned stimulus due to training

eyeblink conditioning

US: airpuff


UR: eyeblink


CS: tone


CR: eyeblink


method of measuring: monitoring electrical potentials generated by the eye muscles and recorded from surface electrodes on the skin around the eye

skin conductance response (SCR)

US: mild shock or loud noise


UR: mild increase in the SCR


CS: a tone or light


CR: increase in SCR


method of measuring: using electrodes

taste aversion in rats

CS: a novel taste, saccharin-flavored water


US: injection of an illness-inducing drug


UR: rats' sickness


CR: rats' sickness


method of measuring: CS is re-presented to assess the degree of conditioning

forward-delayed conditioning

the CS precedes the onset of the US and termination of the CS is delayed until the US is presented; very effective

forward trace conditioning

the CS is a discrete even that is presented and terminated before the US is presented; very effective if the inter-stimuli period is short

simultaneous conditioning

the CS and US are presented at exactly the same time; very weak because there is no time for the subject to anticipate the US

backward conditioning

the US is presented and terminated before the presentation of the CS; not very effective, except in taste aversion

CS-US interval

The CS being presented slightly before the US is the most effect, and maximum conditioning results when the CS-US interval is approximately 200 milliseconds; except with taste aversion

CS-US relevance, belongingness

the idea that certain combinations seem to belong or go together while others do not

Garcia and Koelling study

Rats were presented with a compound CS consisting of a tone, a light and a taste. This was arranged by allowing the rats to lick saccharin-flavored water from a drinking tube. The results showed that the rats that had been ill after exposure to the compound CS refused to drink the saccharin

compound conditioning procedure

when two or more conditioned stimuli occur together before the US, each may become conditioned but to varying degrees

blocking

occurs when the CS is presented in compound with another CS that has already been trained with the US; the already trained CS blocks conditioning to the new CS

positive conitingency

two stimuli tend to occur together and neither tends to occur when the other is absent

negative contingency

one stimulus regularly precedes the absence of another stimulus that is present at other times

latent inhibition

pre-exposures to a CS alone can hinder later conditioning involving that CS

second-order conditioning

when conditioning results from the pairing of a novel CS with a previously conditioned CS

sensory preconditioning

first involves the pairing of two CSs, later, when one of the stimuli is paired with a US and becomes capable of producing a CR, the organism reacts to the other CS as if it to, had been paired with the US

extinction

the decrease and eventual disappearance of a conditioned response once the CS is no longer a reliable signal for a US

stimulus response theory (S-R theory)

the CS becomes merely associated with the UR; the CS comes to elicit the UR or a portion of the UR

stimulus-stimulus theory (S-S theory)


conditioning produces an association between the CS and the US