• Shuffle
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Alphabetize
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Front First
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Both Sides
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
  • Read
    Toggle On
    Toggle Off
Reading...
Front

Card Range To Study

through

image

Play button

image

Play button

image

Progress

1/396

Click to flip

Use LEFT and RIGHT arrow keys to navigate between flashcards;

Use UP and DOWN arrow keys to flip the card;

H to show hint;

A reads text to speech;

396 Cards in this Set

  • Front
  • Back
Absolute threshold
The minimum stimulus energy necessary for an observer to detect a stimulus.
Action
Motor activities such as moving the head or eyes and locomoting through the environment. Action is one of the major outcomes of the perceptual process.
Bottom-up processing
Processing that is based on the information on the receptors. Also called data-based processing.
Classical psychophysical methods
The methods of limits adjustment and constant stimuli described by Fechner that are used for measuring thresholds.
Cognitive influences on perception
How the knowledge memories and expectations that a person brings to a situation influence his or her perception.
Difference threshold
The minimal detectable difference between two stimuli.
Environmental stimulus
The stimulus “out there” in the external environment.
Frontal lobe
Receiving signals from all of the senses the frontal lobe plays an important role in perceptions that involve the coordination of information received through two or more senses. It also serves functions such as language thought memory and motor functioning.
Knowledge
Any information that the perceiver brings to a situation. See also Top-down processing.
Magnitude estimation
A psychophysical method in which the subject assigns numbers to a stimulus that are proportional to the subjective magnitude of the stimulus.
Method of adjustment
A psychophysical method in which the experimenter or the observer adjusts the stimulus intensity in a continuous manner until the observer detects the stimulus.
Method of constant stimuli
A psychophysical method in which a number of stimuli with different intensities are presented repeatedly in a random order.
Method of limits
A psychophysical method for measuring threshold in which the experimenter presents sequences of stimuli in ascending and descending order.
Neural processing
Operations that transform electrical signals within a network of neurons or that transform the response of individual neurons.
Oblique effect
Enhanced sensitivity to vertically and horizontally oriented visual stimuli compared to obliquely oriented (slanted) stimuli. This effect has been demonstrated by measuring both perception and neural responding.
Occipital lobe
A lobe at the back of the cortex that is the site of the cortical receiving area for vision.
Parietal lobe
A lobe at the top of the cortex that is the site of the cortical receiving area for touch and is the termination point of the dorsal (where or how) stream for visual processing.
Perceived magnitude
A perceptual measure of stimuli such as light or sound that indicates the magnitude of experience.
Perception
Conscious sensory experience.
Perceptual process
A sequence of steps leading from the environment to perception of a stimulus recognition of the stimulus and action with regard to the stimulus.
Phenomenological method
Method of determining the relationship between stimuli and perception in which the observer describes what he or she perceives.
Physiological approach to perception
Analyzing perception by determining how a person’s perception is related to physiological processes that are occurring within the person. This approach focuses on determining the relationship between stimuli and physiological responding and between physiological responding and perception.
Power function
A mathematical function of the form P=KSn where P is perceived magnitude K is a constant S is the stimulus intensity and n is an exponent.
Primary receiving areas
Areas of the cerebral cortex that first receive most of the signals initiated by a sense’s receptors. For example the occipital cortex is the site of the primary receiving area for vision and the temporal lobe is the site of the primary receiving area for hearing.
Principle of representation
A principle of perception that everything a person perceives is based not on direct contact with stimuli but on representations of stimuli on the receptors and in the person’s nervous system.
Principle of transformation
A principle of perception that stimuli and responses created by stimuli are transformed or changed between the environmental stimulus and perception.
Psychophysical approach to perception
Analyzing perception by determining how a person’s perception is related to stimuli in the environment. This approach focuses on determining the relationship between stimuli in the environment and perceptual responding.
Psychophysics
Traditionally the term psychophysics refers to quantitative methods for measuring the relationship between properties of the stimulus and the subject’s experience. In this book all methods that are used to determine the relationship between stimuli and perception will be broadly referred to as pychophysical methods.
Rat-man demonstration
The demonstration in which presentation of a “ratlike’’ or “manlike’’ picture influences an observer’s perception of a second picture which can be interpreted either as a rat or as a man. This demonstration illustrates an effect of top-down processing on perception.
Reaction time
The time between presentation of a stimulus and an observer's or listener's response to the stimulus. Reaction time is often used in experiments as a measure of speed of processing.
Recognition
The ability to place an object in a category that gives it meaning—for example recognizing a particular red object as a tomato.
Response compression
The result when doubling the physical intensity of a stimulus less than doubles the subjective magnitude of the stimulus.
Response criterion
In a signal detection experiment the subjective magnitude of a stimulus above which the participant will indicate that the stimulus is present.
Response expansion
The result when doubling the physical intensity of a stimulus more than doubles the subjective magnitude of the stimulus.
Sensory receptors
Cells specialized to respond to environmental energy with each sensory system’s receptors specialized to respond to a specific type of energy.
Signal detection theory
A theory stating that the detection of a stimulus depends both on the participant's sensitivity to the stimulus and on the participant’s response criterion.
Steven's power law
A law concerning the relationship between the physical intensity of a stimulus and the perception of the subjective magnitude of the stimulus. The law states that P=KSn where P is perceived magnitude K is a constant S is the stimulus intensity and n is an exponent.
Temporal lobe
A lobe on the side of the cortex that is the site of the cortical receiving area for hearing and the termination point for the ventral or what stream for visual processing. A number of areas in the temporal lobe such as the fusiform face area and the extrastriate body area serve functions related to perceiving and recognizing objects.
Top-down processing
Processing that starts with the analysis of high-level information such as the knowledge a person brings to a situation. Also called knowledge-based processing. Distinguished from bottom-up or data-based processing which is based on incoming data.
Transduction
In the senses the transformation of environmental energy into electrical energy. For example the retinal receptors transduce light energy into electrical energy.
Visual form agnosia
The inability to recognize objects.
Visual pigment
A light-sensitive molecule contained in the rod and cone outer segments. The reaction of this molecule to light results in the generation of an electrical response in the receptors.
Visual search
A procedure in which a person's task is to find a particular element in a display that contains a number of elements.
Weber fraction
The ratio of the difference threshold to the value of the standard stimulus in Weber’s law.
Weber's law
A law stating that the ratio of the difference threshold (DL) to the value of the stimulus (S) is constant. According to this relationship doubling the value of a stimulus will cause a doubling of the difference threshold. The ratio DL/S is called the Weber fraction.
Absorption spectrum
A plot of the amount of light absorbed by a visual pigment versus the wavelength of light.
Accommodation
In vision bringing objects located at different distances into focus by changing the shape of the lens.
Action potential
Rapid increase in positive charge in a nerve fiber that travels down the fiber. Also called the nerve impulse.
Amacrine cell
A neuron that transmits signals laterally in the retina. Amacrine cells synapse with bipolar cells and ganglion cells.
Axial myopia
Myopia (nearsightedness) in which the eyeball is too long. See also refractive myopia.
Axon
The part of the neuron that conducts nerve impulses over distances. Also called the nerve fiber.
Bipolar cell
A retinal neuron that receives inputs from the visual receptors and sends signals to the retinal ganglion cells.
Blind spot
The small area where the optic nerve leaves the back of the eye. There are no visual receptors in this area so small images falling directly on the blind spot cannot be seen.
Cell body
The part of a neuron that contains the neuron's metabolic machinery and that receives stimulation from other neurons.
Cone
Cone-shaped receptors in the retina that are primarily responsible for vision in high levels of illumination and for color vision and detail vision.
Cone spectral sensitivity curve
A plot of visual sensitivity versus wavelength for cone vision. Often measured by presenting a small spot of light to the fovea which contains only cones. Can also be measured when the eye is light adapted so cones are the most sensitive receptors.
Convergence (neural)
When many neurons synapse onto a single neuron.
Cornea
The transparent focusing element of the eye that is the first structure through which light passes as it enters the eye. The cornea is the eye's major focusing element.
Dark adaptation
Visual adaptation that occurs in the dark during which the sensitivity to light increases. This increase in sensitivity is associated with regeneration of the rod and cone visual pigments.
Dark adaptation curve
The function that traces the time course of the increase in visual sensitivity that occurs during dark adaptation.
Dark-adapted sensitivity
The sensitivity of the eye after it has completely adapted to the dark.
Dendrites
Nerve processes on the cell body that receive stimulation from other neurons.
Depolarization
When the inside of a neuron becomes more positive as occurs during the initial phases of the action potential. Depolarization is often associated with the action of excitatory neurotransmitters.
Detached retina
A condition in which the retina is detached from the back of the eye.
Electromagnetic spectrum
Continuum of electromagnetic energy that extends from very-short-wavelength gamma rays to longwavelength radio waves. Visible light is a narrow band within this spectrum.
Excitatory response
The response of a nerve fiber in which the firing rate increases.
Eye
The eyeball and its contents which include focusing elements the retina and supporting structures.
Falling phase of the action potential
In the axon or nerve fiber the increase in negativity from +40 mV back to –70 mV (the resting potential level) that occurs during the action potential. This increase in negativity is associated with the flow of positively charged potassium ions (K+) out of the axon.
Far point
As a light is moved toward the eye the distance at which the light becomes focused on the retina.
Farsightedness
A condition causing poor vision in which people can see objects that are far away but do not see near objects clearly. Also called hyperopia.
Fovea
A small area in the human retina that contains only cone receptors. The fovea is located on the line of sight so that when a person looks at an object the center of its image falls on the fovea.
Ganglion cell
A neuron in the retina that receives inputs from bipolar and amacrine cells. The axons of the ganglion cells are the nerve fibers that travel out of the eye in the optic nerve.
Horizontal cell
A neuron that transmits signals laterally across the retina. Horizontal cells synapse with receptors and bipolar cells.
Hyperopia
A condition causing poor vision in which people can see objects that are far away but do not see near objects clearly. Also called farsightedness.
Hyperpolarization
When the inside of a neuron becomes more negative. Hyperpolarization is often associated with the action of inhibitory neurotransmitters.
Inhibitory response
Occurs when a neuron's firing rate decreases due to inhibition from another neuron.
Ions
Charged molecules. Sodium (Na+) potassium (K+) and chlorine (Cl–) are the main ions found within nerve fibers and in the liquid that surrounds nerve fibers.
Isomerization
Change in shape of the retinal part of the visual pigment molecule that occurs when the molecule absorbs a quantum of light. Isomerization triggers the enzyme cascade that results in transduction from light energy to electrical energy in the retinal receptors.
Laser-assisted in situ keratomileusis (LASIK)
A process in which the cornea is sculpted with a laser in order to achieve clear vision by adjusting the focusing power of the cornea so it focuses light onto the retina.
Lens
The transparent focusing element of the eye through which light passes after passing through the cornea and the aqueous humor. The lens's change in shape to focus at different distances is called accommodation.
Light-adapted sensitivity
The sensitivity of the eye when in the light-adapted state. Usually taken as the starting point for the dark adaptation curve because it is the sensitivity of the eye just before the lights are turned off.
Macular degeneration
A clinical condition that causes degeneration of the macula an area of the retina that includes the fovea and a small surrounding area.
Monochromatic light
Light that contains only a single wavelength.
Myopia
An inability to see distant objects clearly. Also called nearsightedness.
Near point
The distance at which the lens can no longer accommodate enough to bring close objects into focus. Objects nearer than the near point can be brought into focus only by corrective lenses.
Nearsightedness
An inability to see distant objects clearly. Also called myopia.
Nerve fiber
In most sensory neurons the long part of the neuron that transmits electrical impulses from one point to another. Also called the axon.
Neural circuit
A number of neurons that are connected by synapses.
Neural convergence
Synapsing of a number of neurons onto one neuron.
Neuron
The structure that transmits electrical signals in the body. Key components of neurons are the cell body dendrites and the axon or nerve fiber.
Neurotransmitter
A chemical stored in synaptic vesicles that is released in response to a nerve impulse and has an excitatory or inhibitory effect on another neuron.
Optic nerve
Bundle of nerve fibers that carry impulses from the retina to the lateral geniculate nucleus and other structures. Each optic nerve contains about 1 million ganglion cell fibers.
Outer segment
Part of the rod and cone visual receptors that contain the light-sensitive visual pigment molecules.
Peripheral retina
The area of retina outside the fovea.
Permeability
A property of a membrane that refers to the ability of molecules to pass through it. If the permeability to a molecule is high the molecule can easily pass through the membrane.
Preferential looking (PL) technique
A technique used to measure perception in infants. Two stimuli are presented and the infant’s looking behavior is monitored for the amount of time the infant spends viewing each stimulus.
Presbyopia
The inability of the eye to accommodate due to a hardening of the lens and a weakening of the ciliary muscles. It occurs as people get older.
Propagated response
A response such as a nerve impulse that travels all the way down the nerve fiber without decreasing in amplitude.
Pupil
The opening through which light reflected from objects in the environment enters the eye.
Purkinje shift
The shift from cone spectral sensitivity to rod spectral sensitivity that takes place during dark adaptation. See also spectral sensitivity.
Receptor site
Small area on the postsynaptic neuron that is sensitive to specific neurotransmitters.
Refractive myopia
Myopia (nearsightedness) in which the cornea and/or the lens bends the light too much. See also axial myopia.
Refractory period
The time period of about 1/1000th of a second that a nerve fiber needs to recover from conducting a nerve impulse. No new nerve impulses can be generated in the fiber until the refractory period is over.
Resting potential
The difference in charge between the inside and the outside of the nerve fiber when the fi ber is not conducting electrical signals. Most nerve fi bers have resting potentials of about 70 mV which means the inside of the fiber is negative relative to the outside.
Retina
A complex network of cells that covers the inside back of the eye. These cells include the receptors which generate an electrical signal in response to light as well as the horizontal bipolar amacrine and ganglion cells.
Retinitis pigmentosa
A retinal disease that causes a gradual loss of vision beginning in the peripheral retina.
Rising phase of the action potential
In the axon or nerve fiber the decrease in negativity from –70 mV to +40 mV (the peak action potential level) that occurs during the action potential. This increase is caused by an inflow of Na+ ions into the axon.
Rod
A cylinder-shaped receptor in the retina that is responsible for vision at low levels of illumination.
Rod monochromat
A person who has a retina in which the only functioning receptors are rods.
Rod spectral sensitivity curve
The curve plotting visual sensitivity versus wavelength for rod vision. This function is typically measured when the eye is dark adapted by a test light presented to the peripheral retina.
Rod-cone break
The point on the dark adaptation curve at which vision shifts from cone vision to rod vision.
Sensory receptors
Cells specialized to respond to environmental energy with each sensory system's receptors specialized to respond to a specific type of energy.
Spectral sensitivity
The sensitivity of visual receptors to different parts of the visible spectrum. See also spectral sensitivity curve.
Spectral sensitivity curve
The function relating a subject’s sensitivity to light to the wavelength of the light. The spectral sensitivity curves for rod and cone vision indicate that the rods and cones are maximally sensitive at 500 nm and 560 nm respectively. See also Purkinje shift.
Spontaneous activity
Nerve firing that occurs in the absence of environmental stimulation.
Synapse
A small space between the end of one neuron (the presynaptic neuron) and the cell body of another neuron (the postsynaptic neuron).
Transduction
In the senses the transformation of environmental energy into electrical energy. For example the retinal receptors transduce light energy into electrical energy.
Visible light
The band of electromagnetic energy that activates the visual system and that therefore can be perceived. For humans visible light has wavelengths between 400 and 700 nanometers.
Visual acuity
The ability to resolve small details.
Visual evoked potential (VEP)
An electrical response to visual stimulation recorded by the placement of disk electrodes on the back of the head. This potential reflects the activity of a large population of neurons in the visual cortex.
Visual pigment
A light-sensitive molecule contained in the rod and cone outer segments. The reaction of this molecule to light results in the generation of an electrical response in the receptors.
Visual pigment bleaching
The change in the color of a visual pigment that occurs when visual pigment molecules are isomerized by exposure to light.
Visual pigment regeneration
Occurs after the visual pigment’s two components—opsin and retinal—have become separated due to the action of light. Regeneration which occurs in the dark involves a rejoining of these two components to reform the visual pigment molecule. This process depends on enzymes located in the pigment epithelium.
Wavelength
For light energy the distance between one peak of a light wave and the next peak.
Area V1
The visual receiving area of the brain called area V1 to indicate that it is the first visual area in the cortex. Also called striate cortex.
Belongingness
The hypothesis that an area’s appearance is influenced by the part of the surroundings that the area appears to belong to. This principle has been used to explain the perception of lightness in the Benary cross and White’s illusion.
Center-surround antagonism
the competition between the center and surround regions of a center-surround receptive field caused by the fact that one is excitatory and the other is inhibitory. Stimulating center and surround areas simultaneously decreases responding of the neuron compared to stimulating the excitatory area alone
Center-surround organization
Arrangement of a neuron’s receptive fields in which one area is surrounded by another area like the hole in a donut (corresponding to the center) and the donut (the surround). Stimulation of the center and surround causes opposite responses. See also Excitatory-center inhibitory-surround receptive field; Inhibitory-center excitatory-surround receptive field.
Center-surround receptive field
A receptive field that has a center-surround organization.
Cerebral cortex
The 2-mm-thick layer that covers the surface of the brain and contains the machinery for creating perception as well as for other functions such as language memory and thinking.
Complex cell
A neuron in the visual cortex that responds best to moving bars with a particular orientation.
Contrast threshold
The intensity difference that can just barely be seen between two areas. For vision this is often measured using gratings with alternating light and dark bars.
Distributed coding
Type of neural code in which different perceptual qualities are signaled by the pattern of activity across many neurons. This contrasts with specificity coding in which qualities are signaled by activity in a specific type of neuron.
Easy problem of consciousness
The problem of determining the relationship between physiological processes like nerve firing and perceptual experience. Note that this involves determining a relationship not a cause. See also hard problem of consciousness.
End-stopped cell
A cortical neuron that responds best to lines of a specific length that are moving in a particular direction.
Excitatory area
Area of a receptive field that is associated with excitation. Stimulation of this area causes an increase in the rate of nerve firing.
Excitatory-center
inhibitory-surround receptive field "A center-surround receptive field in which stimulation of the center area causes an excitatory response and stimulation of the surround causes an inhibitory response.
Experience-dependent plasticity
A process by which neurons adapt to the specific environment within which a person or animal lives. This is achieved when neurons change their response properties so they become tuned to respond best to stimuli that have been repeatedly experienced in the environment. See also neural plasticity; selective rearing.
Feature detector
A neuron that responds selectively to a specific feature of the stimulus such as orientation or direction of motion.
Fusiform face area (FFA)
An area in the human inferotemporal (IT) cortex that contains neurons that are specialized to respond to faces.
Grandmother cell
A hypothesized type of neuron that responds only to a very specific stimulus such as a person’s grandmother. See also specificity coding.
Hard problem of consciousness
The problem of determining how physiological processes such as ion flow across nerve membranes cause different perceptual experiences. See also mind-body problem.
Hermann grid
A display that results in the illusion of dark areas at the intersection of two white “corridors.” This perception can be explained by lateral inhibition.
Inferotemporal (IT) cortex
An area of the brain outside Area V1 (the striate cortex) involved in object perception and facial recognition.
Inhibitory area
Area of a receptive field that is associated with inhibition. Stimulation of this area causes a decrease in the rate of nerve firing.
Inhibitory-center
excitatory-surround receptive fieldA center-surround receptive field in which stimulation of the center causes an inhibitory response and stimulation of the surround causes an excitatory response.
Lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN)
The nucleus in the thalamus that receives inputs from the optic nerve and in turn communicates with the cortical receiving area for vision.
Lateral inhibition
Inhibition that is transmitted laterally across a nerve circuit. In the retina lateral inhibition is transmitted by the horizontal and amacrine cells.
Lightness
The perception of shades ranging from white to grey to black.
Mind-body problem
One of the most famous problems in science: How do physical processes such as nerve impulses or sodium and potassium molecules flowing across membranes (the body part of the problem) become transformed into the richness of perceptual experience (the mind part of the problem)? See also hard problem of consciousness.
Neural correlate of consciousness (NCC)
Connections between the firing of neurons and perceptual experience. See also easy problem of consciousness.
Neural plasticity
The capacity of the nervous system to change in response to experience. Examples are how early visual experience can change the orientation selectivity of neurons in the visual cortex and how tactile experience can change the sizes of areas in the cortex that represent different parts of the body. See also experience-dependent plasticity; selective rearing.
Neural processing
Operations that transform electrical signals within a network of neurons or that transform the response of individual neurons.
Occipital lobe
A lobe at the back of the cortex that is the site of the cortical receiving area for vision.
Ommatidium
A structure in the eye of the Limulus that contains a small lens located directly over a visual receptor. The Limulus eye is made up of hundreds of these ommatidia. The Limulus eye has been used for research on lateral inhibition because its receptors are large enough so that stimulation can be applied to individual receptors.
Orientation tuning curve
A function relating the firing rate of a neuron to the orientation of the stimulus.
Prosopagnosia
A form of visual agnosia in which the person can’t recognize faces.
Receptive field
A neuron’s receptive field is the area on the receptor surface (the retina for vision; the skin for touch) that when stimulated affects the firing of that neuron.
Selective adaptation
A procedure in which a person or animal is selectively exposed to one stimulus and then the effect of this exposure is assessed by testing with a wide range of stimuli. Typically sensitivity to the exposed stimulus is decreased.
Selective rearing
A procedure in which animals are reared in special environments. An example of selective rearing is the experiment in which kittens were reared in an environment of vertical stripes to determine the effect on orientation selectivity of cortical neurons.
Simple cortical cells
A neuron in the visual cortex that responds best to bars of a particular orientation.
Simultaneous contrast
The effect that occurs when surrounding one color with another changes the appearance of the surrounded color. Occurs for chromatic and achromatic stimuli.
Sparse coding
The idea that a particular object is represented by the firing of a relatively small number of neurons.
Specifity coding
Type of neural code in which different perceptions are signaled by activity in specific neurons. See also distributed coding.
Striate cortex
The visual receiving area of the cortex located in the occipital lobe.
Superior colliculus
An area in the brain that is involved in controlling eye movements and other visual behaviors. This area receives about 10 percent of the ganglion cell fibers that leave the eye in the optic nerve.
Visual receiving area
The area of the occipital lobe where signals from the retina and LGN first reach the cortex.
White's illusion
A display in which two rectangles are perceived as differing in lightness even though they both reflect the same amount of light and even though the rectangle that is perceived as lighter receives more lateral inhibition than the one perceived as darker.
Ablation
Removal of an area of the brain. This is usually done in experiments on animals to determine the function of a particular area. Also called lesioning.
Action pathway
Pathway that conducts signals from the striate cortex to the parietal lobe. See also dorsal pathway.
Brain imaging
Procedures that make it possible to visualize areas of the human brain that are activated by different types of stimuli tasks or behaviors. The two most common techniques used in perception research are positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).
Cortical magnification
Occurs when a disproportionately large area on the cortex is activated by stimulation of a small area on the receptor surface. One example of cortical magnification is the relatively large area of visual cortex that is activated by stimulation of the fovea. An example in the somatosensory system is the large area of somatosensory cortex activated by stimulation of the lips and fingers.
Dorsal pathway
Pathway that conducts signals from the striate cortex to the parietal lobe. The dorsal pathway has also been called the where the how or the action pathway by different investigators.
Double dissociation
In brain damage when function A is present and function B is absent in one person and function A is absent and function B is present in another. Presence of a double dissociation means that the two functions involve different mechanisms and operate independently of one another.
Expertise hypothesis
The idea that human proficiency in perceiving certain things can be explained by changes in the brain caused by long exposure practice or training.
Extrastriate body area (EBA)
An area of the temporal lobe that is activated by pictures of bodies and parts of bodies.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
A brain imaging technique that indicates brain activity in awake behaving organisms. The fMRI response occurs when the response to a magnetic field changes in response to changes in blood flow in the brain.
Fusiform face area (FFA)
An area in the human inferotemporal (IT) cortex that contains neurons that are specialized to respond to faces.
How pathway
Pathway that conducts signals from the striate cortex to the parietal lobe. See also dorsal pathway.
Hypercolumn
In the striate cortex unit proposed by Hubel and Wiesel that combines location orientation and ocular dominance columns that serve a specific area on the retina.
Landmark discrimination problem
The behavioral task used in Ungerleider and Mishkin’s experiment in which they provided evidence for the dorsal or where visual processing stream. Monkeys were required to respond to a previously indicated location.
Location column
A column in the visual cortex that contains neurons with the same receptive field locations on the retina.
Module
A structure that processes information about a specific behavior or perceptual quality. Often identified as a structure that contains a large proportion of neurons that respond selectively to a particular quality such as the fusiform face area which contains many neurons that respond selectively to faces.
Neuropsychology
The study of the behavioral effects of brain damage in humans.
Object discrimination problem
The behavioral task used in Ungerleider and Mishkin’s experiment in which they provided evidence for the ventral or what visual processing stream. Monkeys were required to respond to an object with a particular shape.
Ocular dominance
The degree to which a neuron is influenced by stimulation of each eye. A neuron has a large amount of ocular dominance if it responds only to stimulation of one eye. There is no ocular dominance if the neuron responds equally to stimulation of both eyes.
Ocular dominance column
A column in the visual cortex that contains neurons that respond best to stimulation of the same eye.
Orientation column
A column in the visual cortex that contains neurons with the same orientation preference.
Parahippocampal place area (PPA)
An area in the temporal lobe that is activated by indoor and outdoor scenes.
Positron emission tomography (PET)
A brain mapping technique that is used in awake human subjects to determine which brain areas are activated by various tasks.
Retinotopic map
A map on a structure in the visual system such as the lateral geniculate nucleus or the cortex that indicates locations on the structure that correspond to locations on the retina. In retinotopic maps locations adjacent to each other on the retina are usually represented by locations that are adjacent to each other on the structure.
Spatial organization
How different locations in the environment and on the receptors are represented in the brain.
Tiling
The adjacent (and often overlapping) location columns working together to cover the entire visual field (similar to covering a floor with tiles).
Ventral pathway
Pathway that conducts signals from the striate cortex to the temporal lobe. Also called the what pathway because it is involved in recognizing objects.
What pathway
Pathway that conducts signals from the striate cortex to the temporal lobe. Called the what pathway because it is involved in recognizing objects. See also ventral pathway.
Where pathway
Pathway that conducts signals from the striate cortex to the parietal lobe. See also dorsal pathway.
Apparent movement
An illusion of movement that occurs when two objects separated in space are presented rapidly one after another separated by a brief time interval.
Bayesian inference
A statistical approach to perception in which perception is determined by taking probabilities into account. These probabilities are based on past experiences in perceiving properties of objects and scenes.
Binocular rivalry
A situation in which one image is presented to the left eye and a different image is presented to the right eye and perception alternates back and forth between the two images.
Border ownership
When two areas share a border as occurs in figure–ground displays the border is usually perceived as belonging to the figure.
Figure
When an object is seen as separate from the background (the “ground”) it is called a figure. See also figure–ground segregation.
Figure-ground segregation
The perceptual separation of an object from its background.
Gestalt psychology
An approach to psychology that developed as a reaction to structuralism. The Gestalt approach proposes principles of perceptual organization and figure–ground segregation and states that “the whole is different than the sum of its parts.”
Gist of a scene
General description of a scene. People can identify most scenes after viewing them for only a fraction of a second as when they flip rapidly from one TV channel to another. It takes longer to identify the details within the scene.
Global image features
Information that may enable observers to rapidly perceive the gist of a scene. Features associated with specific types of scenes include degree of naturalness degree of openness degree of roughness degree of expansion and color.
Ground
In object perception the background is called the ground. See also figure.
Grouping
In perceptual organization the process by which visual events are “put together” into units or objects.
Illusory contour
Contour that is perceived even though it is not present in the physical stimulus.
Inverse projection problem
The idea that a particular image on the retina could have been caused by an infinite number of different objects. This means that the retinal image does not unambiguously specify a stimulus.
Light-from-above assumption
The assumption that light usually comes from above which influences our perception of form in some situations.
Likelihood principle
The idea proposed by Helmholtz that we perceive the object that is most likely to have caused the pattern of stimuli we have received.
Organizing principles
In Gestalt psychology the rules that determine how elements in a scene become grouped together.
Perceptual organization
The process by which small elements become perceptually grouped into larger objects.
Perceptual segregation
Perceptual organization in which one object is seen as separate from other objects.
Persistence of vision
A phenomenon in which perception of any stimulus persists for about 250 ms after the stimulus is physically terminated.
Physical regularities
Regularly occurring physical properties of the environment. For example there are more vertical and horizontal orientations in the environment than oblique (angled) orientations.
Principle of common fate
A Gestalt principle of perceptual organization that states that things that are moving in the same direction appear to be grouped together.
Principle of common region
A modern Gestalt principle that states that elements that are within the same region of space appear to be grouped together.
Principle of good continuation
A Gestalt principle of perceptual organization that states that points that when connected result in straight or smoothly curving lines are seen as belonging together and that lines tend to be seen in such a way as to follow the smoothest path.
Principle of good figure
A Gestalt principle of perceptual organization that states that every stimulus pattern is seen in such a way that the resulting structure is as simple as possible. Also called the principle of pragnanz or the principle of simplicity.
Principle of pragnanz
A Gestalt principle of perceptual organization that states that every stimulus pattern is seen in such a way that the resulting structure is as simple as possible. Also called the principle of good figure or the principle of simplicity.
Principle of proximity
A Gestalt principle of perceptual organization that states that things that are near to each other appear to be grouped together. Also called the law of nearness.
Principle of similarity
A Gestalt principle stating that similar things appear to be grouped together.
Principle of simplicity
A Gestalt principle of perceptual organization that states that every stimulus pattern is seen in such a way that the resulting structure is as simple as possible. Also called the principle of good figure or the principle of pragnanz.
Principle of uniform connectedness
A modern Gestalt principle that states that connected regions of a visual stimulus are perceived as a single unit.
Regularities in the environment
Characteristics of the environment that occur regularly and in many different situations.
Reversible figure-ground
A figure–ground pattern that perceptually reverses as it is viewed so that the figure becomes the ground and the ground becomes the figure. The best-known reversible figure–ground pattern is Rubin’s vase–face pattern.
Scene
A view of a real-world environment that contains (a) background elements and (b) multiple objects that are organized in a meaningful way relative to each other and the background.
Segregation
The process of separating one area or object from another. See also figure–ground segregation.
Semantic encoding
A method for analyzing the patterns of voxel activation recorded from visual areas of an observer’s brain based on the relationship between voxel activation and the meaning or category of a scene.
Semantic regularities
Characteristics associated with the functions associated with different types of scenes. These characteristics are learned from experience. For example most people are aware of the kinds of activities and objects that are usually associated with kitchens.
Sensations
Elementary elements that according to the structuralists combine to create perceptions.
Structural encoding
A method for analyzing the patterns of voxel activation recorded from visual areas of an observer’s brain based on the relationship between voxel activation and structural characteristics of a scene such as lines contrasts shapes and textures.
Structuralism
The approach to psychology prominent in the late 19th and early 20th centuries that postulated that perceptions result from the summation of many elementary sensations. The Gestalt approach to perception was in part a reaction to structuralism.
Theory of unconscious inference
The idea proposed by Helmholtz that some of our perceptions are the result of unconscious assumptions that we make about the environment. See also likelihood principle.
Viewpoint invariance
The condition in which object properties don’t change when viewed from different angles. Responsible for our ability to recognize objects when viewed from different angles.
Visual masking stimulus
A visual pattern that when presented immediately after a visual stimulus decreases a person’s ability to perceive the stimulus. This stops the persistence of vision and therefore limits the effective duration of the stimulus.
Attention
The process of focusing on some objects while ignoring others. Attention can enhance the processing of the attended object.
Attentional capture
Occurs when stimulus salience causes an involuntary shift of attention. For example attention can be captured by movement.
Autism
A serious developmental disorder in which one of the major symptoms is the withdrawal of contact from other people. People with autism typically do not make eye contact with others and have difficulty telling what emotions others are experiencing in social situations.
Balint's syndrome
A condition resulting from damage to a person’s parietal lobe. One characteristic of this syndrome is an inability to focus attention on individual objects.
Binding
The process by which features such as color form motion and location are combined to create our perception of a coherent object. Binding can also occur across senses as when sound and vision are associated with the same object.
Binding problem
The problem of how neural activity in many separated areas in the brain is combined to create a perception of a coherent object.
Change blindness
Difficulty in detecting differences between two visual stimuli that are presented one after another often with a short blank stimulus interposed between them. Also occurs when part of a stimulus is changed very slowly.
Conjunction search
A visual search task in which it is necessary to search for a combination (or conjunction) of two or more features on the same stimulus to find the target. An example of a conjunction search would be looking for a horizontal green line among vertical green lines and horizontal red lines.
Covert attention
Attention without looking. Seeing something “out of the corner of your eye” is an example of covert attention.
Dishabituation
An increase in looking time that occurs when a stimulus is changed. This response is used in testing infants to see whether they can differentiate two stimuli.
Dual-task procedure
An experimental procedure in which subjects are required to carry out simultaneously a central task that demands attention and a peripheral task that involves making a decision about the contents of a scene.
Feature integration theory
A theory proposed by Treisman to explain how an object is broken down into features and how these features are recombined to result in a perception of the object.
Feature search
A visual search task in which a person can find a target by searching for only one feature. An example would be looking for a horizontal green line among vertical green lines.
Fixation
The brief pause of the eye that occurs between eye movements as a person scans a scene.
Focused attention stage (of perceptual processing)
The stage of processing in feature integration theory in which the features are combined. According to Treisman this stage requires focused attention.
Habituation
Paying less attention to the same stimulus that is presented repeatedly. For example infants look at a stimulus less and less on each successive trial. See also dishabituation.
High-load task
Task that involves more processing resources and that therefore uses more of a person’s perceptual capacity.
Illusory conjunction
Illusory combination of features that are perceived when stimuli containing a number of features are presented briefly and under conditions in which focused attention is difficult. For example presenting a red square and a blue triangle could potentially create the perception of a red triangle.
Inattentional blindness
A situation in which a stimulus that is not attended is not perceived even though the person is looking directly at it.
Load theory of attention
Lavie’s proposal that the amount of perceptual capacity that remains as a person is carrying out a task determines how well the person can avoid being distracted by task-irrelevant stimuli. If a person’s perceptual load is close to perceptual capacity the person is less likely to be distracted by task-irrelevant stimuli. See also high-load tasks; low-load tasks; perceptual capacity; perceptual load.
Low-load task
A task that uses only a small amount of the person’s perceptual capacity.
Overt attention
Attention that involves looking directly at the attended object.
Perceptual capacity
The resources a person has for carrying out perceptual tasks.
Perceptual completion
The perception of an object as extending behind occluding objects.
Perceptual load
The amount of a person’s perceptual capacity needed to carry out a particular perceptual task.
Preattentive stage (of perceptual processing)
An automatic and rapid stage of processing proposed by Treisman’s feature integration theory during which a stimulus is decomposed into individual features.
Precueing
A procedure in which a cue stimulus is presented to direct an observer’s attention to a specific location where a test stimulus is likely to be presented. This procedure was used by Posner to show that attention enhances the processing of a stimulus presented at the cued location.
Saccadic eye movement
Rapid eye movement between fixations that occurs when scanning a scene.
Saliency map
A “map” of a visual display that takes into account characteristics of the display such as color contrast and orientation that are associated with capturing attention.
Same-object advantage
The faster responding that occurs when enhancement spreads within an object. Faster reaction times occur when a target is located within the object that is receiving the subject’s attention even if the subject is looking at another place within the object.
Scene schema
An observer’s knowledge about what is contained in typical scenes. An observer’s attention is affected by knowledge of what is usually found in the scene.
Scene statistics
The probability of various things occurring in the environment.
Spatial attention
Attention to a specific location.
Stimulus salience
Characteristics such as bright colors high contrast and highly visible orientations that cause stimuli to stand out and therefore attract attention.
Task-irrelevant stimuli
Stimuli that do not provide information relevant to the task at hand.
Visual scanning
Moving the eyes to focus attention on different locations on objects or in scenes.
Visual search
A procedure in which a person’s task is to find a particular element in a display that contains a number of elements.
Akinetopsia
A condition in which damage to an area of the cortex involved in motion perception causes blindness to motion.
Aperture problem
Occurs when only a portion of a moving stimulus can be seen as when the stimulus is viewed through a narrow aperture. This can result in misleading information about the direction in which the stimulus is moving.
Apparent motion
An illusion of movement that occurs when two objects separated in space are presented rapidly one after another separated by a brief time interval.
Attentional capture
Occurs when stimulus salience causes an involuntary shift of attention. For example attention can be captured by movement.
Biological motion
Motion produced by biological organisms. Most of the experiments on biological motion have used walking humans with lights attached to their joints and limbs as stimuli. See also point-light walker.
Coherence
In research on movement perception in which arrays of moving dots are used as stimuli the degree of correlation between the direction of the moving dots. Zero percent coherence means all of the dots are moving independently; 100 percent coherence means all of the dots are moving in the same direction.
Comparator
A structure hypothesized by the corollary discharge theory of movement perception. The corollary discharge signal and the sensory movement signal meet at the comparator to determine whether movement will be perceived.
Corollary discharge signal (CDS)
A copy of the motor signal that is sent to the eye muscles to cause movement of the eye. The copy is sent to the hypothetical comparator of corollary discharge theory.
Corollary discharge theory
The theory that explains motion perception as being determined both by movement of the image on the retina and by signals that indicate movement of the eyes. See also corollary discharge signal.
Event
A segment of time at a particular location that is perceived by observers to have a beginning and an ending.
Event boundary
The point in time when one event ends and another begins.
Global optic flow
Information for movement that occurs when all elements in a scene move. The perception of global optic flow indicates that it is the observer that is moving and not the scene.
Illusory motion
Perception of motion when there actually is none. See also apparent motion.
Image displacement signal (IDS)
In corollary discharge theory the signal that occurs when an image moves across the visual receptors.
Implied motion
When a still picture depicts an action that involves motion so that an observer could potentially extend the action depicted in the picture in his or her mind based on what will most likely happen next.
Induced motion
The illusory movement of one object that is caused by the movement of another object that is nearby.
Local disturbance in the optic array
Occurs when one object moves relative to the environment so that the stationary background is covered and uncovered by the moving object. This local disturbance indicates that the object is moving relative to the environment.
Microstimulation
A procedure in which a small electrode is inserted into the cortex and an electrical current passed through the electrode activates neurons near the tip of the electrode. This procedure has been used to determine how activating specific groups of neurons affects perception.
Motion aftereffect
An illusion that occurs after a person views a moving stimulus and then sees movement in the opposite direction when viewing a stationary stimulus immediately afterward. See also waterfall illusion.
Motor signal (MS)
In corollary discharge theory the signal that is sent to the eye muscles when the observer moves or tries to move his or her eyes.
Optic array
The structured pattern of light created by the presence of objects surfaces and textures in the environment.
Point-light walker
A biological motion stimulus created by placing lights at a number of places on a person’s body and having an observer view the moving-light stimulus that results as the person moves in the dark.
Real motion
The physical movement of a stimulus. Contrasts with apparent motion.
Real-motion neuron
Neuron in the monkey’s cortex that responds when movement of an image across the retina is caused by movement of a stimulus but does not respond when movement across the retina is caused by movement of the eyes.
Reichardt detector
A neural circuit that results in neurons firing to movement in one direction. Excitation and inhibition are arranged so that movement in one direction creates inhibition that reduces or eliminates neural responding whereas movement in the opposite direction creates excitation that enhances neural responding.
Representational momentum
Occurs when motion depicted in a still picture continues in an observer’s mind.
Shortest path constraint
In the perception of apparent motion the principle that apparent movement tends to occur along the shortest path between two stimuli.
Waterfall illusion
An aftereffect of movement that occurs after viewing a stimulus moving in one direction such as a waterfall. Viewing the waterfall makes other objects appear to move in the opposite direction. See also movement aftereffect.
Achromatic color
Color without hue. White black and all the grays between these two extremes are achromatic colors.
Additive color mixture
The creation of colors that occurs when lights of different colors are superimposed.
Anomalous trichromat
A person who needs to mix a minimum of three wavelengths to match any other wavelength in the spectrum but mixes these wavelengths in different proportions from a trichromat.
Cerebral achromatopsia
A loss of color vision caused by damage to the cortex.
Chromatic adaptation
Exposure to light in a specific part of the visible spectrum. This adaptation can cause a decrease in sensitivity to light from the area of the spectrum that was presented during adaptation.
Chromatic color
Color with hue such as blue yellow red or green.
Color blindness
A condition in which a person perceives no chromatic color. This can be caused by absent or malfunctioning cone receptors or by cortical damage.
Color constancy
The effect in which the perception of an object’s hue remains constant even when the wavelength distribution of the illumination is changed. Partial color constancy occurs when our perception of hue changes a little when the illumination changes though not as much as we might expect from the change in the wavelengths of light reaching the eye.
Color deficiency
People with this condition (sometimes incorrectly called color blindness) see fewer colors than people with normal color vision and need to mix fewer wavelengths to match any other wavelength in the spectrum.
Color-matching experiment
A procedure in which observers are asked to match the color in one field by mixing two or more lights in another field.
Desaturated
Low saturation in chromatic colors as would occur when white is added to a color. For example pink is not as saturated as red.
Deuteranopia
A form of red–green color dichromatism caused by lack of the middle-wavelength cone pigment.
Dichromat
A person who has a form of color deficiency. Dichromats can match any wavelength in the spectrum by mixing two other wavelengths. Deuteranopes protanopes and tritanopes are all dichromats.
Double-opponent neurons
Neurons that have receptive fields in which stimulation of one part of the receptive field causes an excitatory response to wavelengths in one area of the spectrum and an inhibitory response to wavelengths in another area of the spectrum and stimulation of an adjacent part of the receptive field causes the opposite response. An example of double-opponent responding is when the response of one part of a receptive field is L+ M- and the response of an adjacent part is L- M+.
Hue
The experience of a chromatic color such as red green yellow or blue or combinations of these colors.
Illumination edge
The border between two areas created by different light intensities in the two areas.
Ishihara plate
A display of colored dots used to test for the presence of color deficiency. The dots are colored so that people with normal (trichromatic) color vision can perceive numbers in the plate but people with color deficiency cannot perceive these numbers or perceive different numbers than someone with trichromatic vision.
Lightness
The perception of shades ranging from white to grey to black.
Lightness constancy
The constancy of our perception of an object’s lightness under different intensities of illumination.
Memory color
The idea that an object’s characteristic color influences our perception of that object’s color.
Metamerism
The situation in which two physically different stimuli are perceptually identical. In vision this refers to two lights with different wavelength distributions that are perceived as having the same color.
Metamers
Two lights that have different wavelength distributions but are perceptually identical.
Monochromat
A person who is completely color-blind and therefore sees everything as black white or shades of gray. A monochromat can match any wavelength in the spectrum by adjusting the intensity of any other wavelength. Monochromats generally have only one type of functioning receptors usually rods.
Neutral point
The wavelength at which a dichromat perceives gray.
Opponent neuron
A neuron that has an excitatory response to wavelengths in one part of the spectrum and an inhibitory response to wavelengths in the other part of the spectrum.
Opponent-process theory of color vision
A theory originally proposed by Hering which claimed that our perception of color is determined by the activity of two opponent mechanisms: a blue–yellow mechanism and a red–green mechanism. The responses to the two colors in each mechanism oppose each other one being an excitatory response and the other an inhibitory response. In addition this theory also includes a black–white mechanism which is concerned with the perception of brightness. See also opponent neuron.
Partial color constancy
A type of color constancy that occurs when changing an object’s illumination causes a change in perception of the object’s hue but less change than would be expected based on the change in the wavelengths of light reaching the eye. Note that in complete color constancy changing an object’s illumination causes no change in the object’s hue.
Penumbra
The fuzzy border at the edge of a shadow.
Protanopia
A form of red–green dichromatism caused by a lack of the long-wavelength cone pigment.
Ratio principle
A principle stating that two areas that reflect different amounts of light will have the same perceived lightness if the ratios of their intensities to the intensities of their surroundings are the same.
Reflectance
The percentage of light reflected from a surface.
Reflectance curve
A plot showing the percentage of light reflected from an object versus wavelength.
Reflectance edge
An edge between two areas where the reflectance of two surfaces changes.
Saturation (color)
The relative amount of whiteness in a chromatic color. The less whiteness a color contains the more saturated it is.
Selective reflection
When an object reflects some wavelengths of the spectrum more than others.
Selective transmission
When some wavelengths pass through visually transparent objects or substances and others do not. Selective transmission is associated with the perception of chromatic color. See also selective reflection.
Simultaneous color contrast
The effect that occurs when surrounding one color with another changes the appearance of the surrounded color. Occurs for chromatic and achromatic stimuli.
Single-opponent neuron
Neurons that increase firing to long wavelengths presented to the center of the receptive field and decrease firing to short wavelengths presented to the surround (or vice versa).
Subtractive color mixture
The creation of colors that occurs when paints of different colors are mixed together.
Trichromat
A person with normal color vision. Trichromats can match any wavelength in the spectrum by mixing three other wavelengths in various proportions.
Trichromatic theory of color vision
A theory proposing that our perception of color is determined by the ratio of activity in three receptor mechanisms with different spectral sensitivities. See also Young-Helmholtz theory of color vision.
Tritanopia
A form of dichromatism thought to be caused by a lack of the short-wavelength cone pigment.
Unilateral dichromat
A person who has dichromatic vision in one eye and trichromatic vision in the other eye. People with this condition (which is extremely rare) have been tested to determine what colors a dichromats perceive by asking them to compare the perceptions they experience with their dichromatic eye and their trichromatic eye.
Young-Helmholtz theory of color vision
A theory proposing that our perception of color is determined by the ratio of activity in three receptor mechanisms with different spectral sensitivities. See also trochromatic theory of color vision.
Absolute disparity
The visual angle between the images of an object on the two retinas. When images of an object fall on corresponding points the angle of disparity is zero. When images fall on noncorresponding points the angle of disparity indicates the degree of noncorrespondence.
Accretion
A cue that provides information about the relative depth of two surfaces. Occurs when the farther object is uncovered by the nearer object due to sideways movement of an observer relative to the objects. See also deletion.
Active method (3-D TV)
A method used to create 3-D television images. The active method alternates the left-eye and right-eye images on the screen 30 or more times a second. This method is called active because the viewing glasses have a shutter system that is synchronized with the alternating images on the TV screen.
Ames room
A distorted room first built by Adelbert Ames that creates an erroneous perception of the sizes of people in the room. The room is constructed so that two people at the far wall of the room appear to stand at the same distance from an observer. In actuality one of the people is much farther away than the other.
Angle of disparity
The visual angle between the images of an object on the two retinas. When images of an object fall on corresponding points the angle of disparity is zero. When images fall on noncorresponding points the angle of disparity indicates the degree of noncorrespondence.
Angular size contrast theory
An explanation of the moon illusion that states that the perceived size of the moon is determined by the sizes of the objects that surround it. According to this idea the moon appears small when it is surrounded by large objects such as the expanse of the sky when the moon is overhead.
Apparent distance theory
An explanation of the moon illusion that is based on the idea that the horizon moon which is viewed across the filled space of the terrain should appear farther away than the zenith moon which is viewed through the empty space of the sky. This theory states that because the horizon and zenith moons have the same visual angle but are perceived to be at different distances the farther appearing horizon moon should appear larger.
Atmospheric perspective
A depth cue. Objects that are farther away look more blurred and bluer than objects that are closer because we must look through more air and particles to see them.
Binocular depth cell
A neuron in the visual cortex that responds best to stimuli that fall on points separated by a specific degree of disparity on the two retinas. Also called a disparity-selective cell.
Binocular disparity
Occurs when the retinal images of an object fall on disparate points on the two retinas.
Binocularly fixate
Directing the two foveas to exactly the same spot.
Conflicting cues theory
A theory of visual illusions proposed by R. H. Day which states that our perception of line length depends on an integration of the actual line length and the overall figure length.
Correspondence problem
The problem faced by the visual system which must determine which parts of the images in the left and right eyes correspond to one another. Another way of stating the problem is: How does the visual system match up the images in the two eyes? This matching of the images is involved in determining depth perception using the cue of binocular disparity.
Corresponding retinal points
The points on each retina that would overlap if one retina were slid on top of the other. Receptors at corresponding points send their signals to the same location in the brain.
Cue approach to depth perception
The approach to explaining depth perception that identifies information in the retinal image and also information provided by aiming and focusing the eyes on an object that is correlated with depth in the scene. Some of the depth cues that have been identified are overlap relative height relative size atmospheric perspective convergence and accommodation.
Deletion
A cue that provides information about the relative depth of two surfaces. Deletion occurs when a farther object is covered by a nearer object due to sideways movement of an observer relative to the objects. See also accretion.
Disparity tuning curve
A plot of a neuron’s response versus the degree of disparity of a visual stimulus. The disparity to which a neuron responds best is an important property of disparity-selective cells which are also called binocular depth cells.
Disparity-selective cell
A neuron in the visual cortex that responds best to stimuli that fall on points separated by a specific degree of disparity on the two retinas. Also called a binocular depth cell.
Echolocation
Locating objects by sending out high-frequency pulses and sensing the echo created when these pulses are reflected from objects in the environment. Echolocation is used by bats and dolphins.
Emmert's law
A law stating that the size of an afterimage depends on the distance of the surface against which the afterimage is viewed. The farther away the surface the larger the afterimage appears.
Familiar size
A depth cue in which judgment of distance is based on knowledge of the sizes of objects. Epstein’s coin experiment illustrated the operation of the cue of familiar size by showing that the relative sizes of the coins infl uenced perception of the coins’ distances.
Frontal eyes
Eyes located in front of the head so the views of the two eyes overlap.
Horopter
An imaginary surface that passes through the point of fixation. Images caused by a visual stimulus on this surface fall on corresponding points on the two retinas.
Lateral eyes
Eyes located on opposite sides of an animal’s head so the views of the two eyes do not overlap or overlap only slightly as in the pigeon and rabbit.
Lenticular projection
A method used to create 3-D television images. The screen is coated with a film containing two sets of lenses which results in different images reaching the left and right eyes.
Müller-Lyer illusion
An illusion in which two lines of equal length appear to be of different lengths because of the addition of “fins’’ to the ends of the lines.
Misapplied size constancy scaling
A principle proposed by Richard Gregory that when mechanisms that help maintain size constancy in the three-dimensional world are applied to two-dimensional pictures an illusion of size sometimes results.
Monocular cue
Depth cue—such as overlap relative size relative height familiar size linear perspective movement parallax and accommodation—that can work when we use only one eye.
Moon illusion
An illusion in which the moon appears to be larger when it is on or near the horizon than when it is high in the sky.
Motion parallax
A depth cue. As an observer moves nearby objects appear to move rapidly across the visual field whereas far objects appear to move more slowly.
Noncorresponding points
Two points one on each retina that would not overlap if the retinas were slid onto each other. Also called disparate points.
Occlusion
Depth cue in which one object hides or partially hides another object from view causing the hidden object to be perceived as being farther away. A monocular depth cue.
Oculomotor cue
Depth cue that depends on our ability to sense the position of our eyes and the tension in our eye muscles. Accommodation and convergence are oculomotor cues.
Passive method (3-D TV)
A method used to create 3-D television images. Polarized light is used to create left and right images—one image is polarized so its vibration is vertical and the other is polarized so its vibration is horizontal. The TV is viewed through polarizing lenses which let vertically polarized light into one eye and horizontally polarized light into the other eye creating the disparity that results in three-dimensional perception.
Perspective convergence
The perception that parallel lines in the distance converge as distance increases.
Pictorial cue
Monocular depth cue such as overlap relative height and relative size that can be depicted in pictures.
Ponzo illusion
An illusion of size in which two objects of equal size that are positioned between two converging lines appear to be different in size. Also called the railroad track illusion.
Random-dot stereogram
A pair of stereoscopic images made up of random dots. When one section of this pattern is shifted slightly in one direction the resulting disparity causes the shifted section to appear above or below the rest of the pattern when the patterns are viewed in a stereoscope.
Relative disparity
The difference between two objects’ absolute disparities.
Relative height
A monocular depth cue. Objects that have bases below the horizon appear to be farther away when they are higher in the field of view. Objects that have bases above the horizon appear to be farther away when they are lower in the field of view.
Relative size
A cue for depth perception. When two objects are of equal size the one that is farther away will take up less of the field of view.
Size constancy
Occurs when the size of an object is perceived to remain the same even when it is viewed from different distances.
Size-distance scaling
A hypothesized mechanism that helps maintain size constancy by taking an object’s perceived distance into account. According to this mechanism an object’s perceived size S is determined by multiplying the size of the retinal image R times the object’s perceived distance D.
Stereopsis
The impression of depth that results from binocular disparity—the difference in the position of images of the same object on the retinas of the two eyes.
Stereoscope
A device that presents pictures to the left and the right eyes so that the binocular disparity a person would experience when viewing an actual scene is duplicated. The result is a convincing illusion of depth.
Stereoscopic depth perception
The perception of depth that is created by input from both eyes. See also binocular disparity.
Stereoscopic vision
Two-eyed depth perception involving mechanisms that take into account differences in the images formed on the left and right eyes.
Strabismus
Misalignment of the eyes such as crossed-eyes or walleyes (outward looking eyes) in which the visual system suppresses vision in one of the eyes to avoid double vision so the person sees the world with only one eye at a time.
Texture gradient
The visual pattern formed by a regularly textured surface that extends away from the observer. This pattern provides information for distance because the elements in a texture gradient appear smaller as distance from the observer increases.
Visual angle
The angle of an object relative to an observer’s eyes. This angle can be determined by extending two lines from the eye—one to one end of an object and the other to the other end of the object. Because an object’s visual angle is always determined relative to an observer its visual angle changes as the distance between the object and the observer changes.